Nhiều nghiên cứu đã khám phá ảnh hưởng khác biệt của lời khen năng lực
và lời khen nỗ lực đến sự tự tin vào năng lực của học sinh nhỏ nhưng còn
hạn chế trên nhóm sinh viên. Đây là một sự thiếu hụt quan trọng bởi việc
khen ngợi và mức độ tự tin vào năng lực có liên quan mật thiết đến quần thể
sinh viên, và sinh viên tuổi trưởng thành so với nhóm học sinh nhỏ tuổi có
sự khác biệt quan trọng trong cách nhìn nhận lời khen năng lực và lời khen
nỗ lực. Hơn nữa, khác biệt văn hóa trong quan điểm về lời khen của người
Việt và người nước ngoài cũng có thể cản trở việc khái quát hóa các phát
hiện trước đó. Vì thế, nghiên cứu này muốn tìm hiểu liệu khen ngợi về năng
lực và nỗ lực có sự liên hệ khác nhau đến mức độ tự tin vào năng lực của giới
trẻ Việt Nam, và liệu mức độ chân thành của lời khen có điều tiết mối quan
hệ này. Tổng cộng 403 sinh viên đại học được khảo sát trực tuyến về mức độ
tự tin vào năng lực tổng quát và năng lực học thuật, tần suất được khen về
năng lực và nỗ lực, và mức độ chân thành của những lời khen đó. Phân tích
mô hình tuyến tính cho thấy, lời khen năng lực là một yếu tố dự báo mạnh
hơn cho sự tự tin vào năng lực. Ngoài ra, mức độ chân thành chỉ điều tiết
đáng kể tần suất lời khen năng lực nhưng không điều tiết tần suất lời khen
nỗ lực. Những kết quả này sau đó được thảo luận về cách các nhà giáo dục
và cha mẹ nên áp dụng sự khen ngợi như một hình thức thuyết phục xã hội
để nâng cao mức độ tự tin vào năng lực của sinh viên.
21 trang |
Chia sẻ: Thục Anh | Ngày: 18/05/2022 | Lượt xem: 348 | Lượt tải: 0
Bạn đang xem trước 20 trang nội dung tài liệu Lời khen năng lực và lời khen nỗ lực đối với sự tự tin vào năng lực của sinh viên, để xem tài liệu hoàn chỉnh bạn click vào nút DOWNLOAD ở trên
little above the
recipient’s ability. Other researchers have also agreed on the importance of
sincerity in praising (Bandura, 1999; Delin & Baumeister, 1994). However,
we found that sincerity did not moderate the effect of frequent effort
praise, which contradicts Henderlong and Lepper (2002) who suggest
that the effects of both ability and effort praise should be moderated by
sincerity. Henderlong and Lepper (2002) argue that if effort praise does
588
not match with one’s level of expended effort, i.e. praising a person for
their hard work when they think that they did not expend much effort, the
praise will be disregarded (Henderlong & Lepper, 2002). Here, we argue
that self-judgment of one’s effort is not as simple as self-judgment of one’s
ability, because effort is a more complex construct for assessment. It might
be possible that one person expends a great deal of effort but perceives
that it is not high enough, while another person who does not put as much
effort as required for the difficulty level of a task but self-judges that their
effort is substantial. In other words, judging effort is like judging a whole
process of trying, which is invisible, intangible, and hard to quantify (e.g.
nights of sleeplessness, personal struggle, and time spent on task), while
judging ability is like judging the end product, which is visible, tangible
and quantifiable (e.g. a final test score). Thus, because there is no clear
benchmark to judge effort, evaluations of effort can be easily influenced
by other people’s evaluations because the evaluators themselves (both the
recipient of praise who evaluates their own effort and the giver of praise
who evaluates other people’s effort) are unsure and unconfident of how
correctly they evaluate the expended effort. If this is the case, then we argue
that both sides (the recipient and the giver) will find an equilibrium to
match their effort evaluations, resulting in praise that always seems sincere.
We found at least two experiments that investigated the effect of
reward on the judgement of effort. In 2015, Pooresmaeili et al. asked
26 adult participants to perform a task requiring physical effort by
continuously pressing two keyboards to push a ball up a virtual ramp.
Half of the participants in the experimental group received an amount
of money equivalent to the level of difficulty they reached, and the other
half in the control group also received a monetary reward but was random
and unrelated to the task difficulty. Their results showed that participants
revised their self-judgement of effort after receiving the rewards, suggesting
that reward magnitude has an effect on self-judgement of effort. When
receiving higher rewards, participants overestimated their effort, with a
converse effect observed for lower rewards. Later, Rollwage et al. (2020)
replicated and extended this research design to investigate whether this
reward magnitude effect still exists for evaluators who judge other people’s
efforts. Using the same experimental paradigm, 51 participants were asked
589
to rate both their own and other participants’ efforts on the same ball task.
The results were parallel with Pooresmaeili et al. (2015), showing that
higher rewards were associated with higher effort rated for both self– and
others-judgement. As praise can be a kind of verbal reward, based on these
studies, we suggest that being praised for high effort by other people makes
one reassesses their own level of effort, whether or not they truly expended
much effort. Thus, frequent effort praise might always work even if it is
exaggerated or understated.
Several limitations in this study will be discussed with suggestions
for future direction. Firstly, because of the retrospective cross-sectional
design, our result still implies a bidirectional relationship between praise
and self-efficacy. While much of our discussion is on how praise can be
a booster of self-efficacy, it is still possible that highly self-efficacious
people tend to receive more praises because they achieve more than low
self-efficacious peers, hence the report of a positive association between
frequency of praise and self-efficacy. In order to make causal conclusion
on the effect of praise, more experiments should be done in the future,
in which participants should be assigned into at least three groups:
receiving ability praise, receiving effort praise, and receiving no praise.
Secondly, future research can explore qualitatively and quantitatively
how Vietnamese perceive ability praise and effort praise differently from
a cultural standpoint. In real-life circumstances, praising is oftentimes a
form of greetings in Vietnamese culture (Ngô Hương Lan, 2016), which
can make it more susceptible to be non-specific and untrustworthy. This
cultural characteristic of Vietnamese people should be considered when
designing studies on praise. Thirdly, the majority of our sample were
students from one university in Ho Chi Minh city and were mostly females,
which may hinder the generalization of our results. Fourthly, because
we did not use back-translation when translating the GSE and CASES
scales, misunderstandings might have occurred that hinder accurate
measurements. Future studies should always use back-translation when
using non-Vietnamese scales. Finally, it would be necessary to explore
whether our findings on praise and self-efficacy are also applied to other
special and underrepresented groups such as Vietnamese students with
learning disabilities (LD). As it has been found that students with LD often
590
underestimate their self-efficacy (Seyed et al., 2017) and social persuasion
is not a significant source of efficacy information (Hampton, 1998), using
praise to persuade that they are capable might not work in the same way
as for typical functioning students. Thus, more research is warranted to
examine how ability and effort praise work differently for Vietnamese
students with LD.
V. CONCLUSION
Our findings support Bandura’s self-efficacy theory that social
persuasion in forms of verbal praise is a significant predictor of self-
efficacy. Here, we found that different types of praise, including ability
and effort praise, have differential and sophisticated associations with self-
efficacy in general and in academic contexts. Furthermore, regardless of
which type of praise is given, sincerity of praise, i.e. the extent to which
the recipient of praise perceives that the praise matches themselves, is an
important factor. As the first study looking at the role of praise on self-
efficacy of Vietnamese university students, we hope that our results give a
few highlights to inform the practice of praising for teachers and parents,
that if ability praise is used, it needs to be frequent and honest, while effort
praise can be either frequent or honest.
REFERENCES
In Vietnamese
Bùi Thị Phương Chi & Phạm Thị Thu Hà (2018). Một vài khảo sát về đặc điểm văn
hóa của người Châu Âu và người Việt thể hiện qua lời khen. Retrieved from:
mot-vai-khao-sat-ve-dac-diem-van-hoa-cua-nguoi-chau-au-va-nguoi-
viet-the-hien-qua-loi-khen
Ngô Hương Lan (2016). Đặc trưng văn hóa ứng xử của người Việt Nam và người
Nhật Bản qua một số hành vi. Đề tài cấp Viện thuộc Viện Nghiên cứu Đông
Bắc Á. Retrieved from:
Nguyễn Văn Quang (1999). Một số khác biệt giao tiếp lời nói Việt – Mỹ
trong cách thức khen và tiếp nhận lời khen. Luận án Tiến Sĩ, Đại
học Quốc gia Hà Nội. Retrieved from:
luanan?a=d&d=TTkGWOSplxFa1999&e=-------vi-20--1--img-
txIN-------#
591
In English
Amemiya, J., & Wang, M. T. (2018). Why effort praise can backfire in adolescence.
Child Development Perspectives, 12(3), 199-203. https://doi.org/10.1111/
cdep.12284
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change.
Psychological Review, 84(2), 191-215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-
295X.84.2.191
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. American
psychologist, 37(2), 122. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.37.2.122
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. W. H. Freeman and
Company.
Bhanji, J. P., & Delgado, M. R. (2014). The social brain and reward: Social
information processing in the human striatum. WIREs Cognitive Science,
5(1), 61-73. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1266
Delin, C. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (1994). Praise: More than just social reinforcement.
Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 24(3), 219-241. https://doi.
org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1994.tb00254.x
Hampton, N. Z. (1998). Sources of academic self-efficacy scale: An assessment
tool for rehabilitation counselors. Rehabilitation Counseling Bulletin,
41(4), 260–277.
Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise on children’s intrinsic
motivation: A review and synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 128(5), 774-
795. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.5.774
Holloway, S. D. (1988). Concepts of ability and effort in Japan and the United
States. Review of Educational Research, 58(3), 327-345. https://doi.
org/10.3102/00346543058003327
Ifdil, I., Bariyyah, K., Dewi, A. K., & Rangka, I. B. (2019). The college academic
self-efficacy scale (CASES): An Indonesian validation to measure the self-
efficacy of students. Jurnal Kajian Bimbingan dan Konseling, 4(4), 115-121.
Jain, S., Bruce, M. A., Stellern, J., & Srivastava, N. (2007). Self-efficacy as a function
of attributional feedback. Journal of School Counseling, 5(4), n4.
Muenks, K., & Miele, D. B. (2017). Students’ thinking about effort and ability:
the role of developmental, contextual, and individual difference
factors. Review of Educational Research, 87(4), 707-735. https://doi.
org/10.3102/0034654316689328
592
Nguyễn Thị Xuân Hồng & Phan Thị Tuyết Nga (2020). Students’ self-efficacy
beliefs and TOEIC achievements in the Vietnamese context. International
Journal of Instruction, 13(4), 67-86. https://doi.org/10.29333/iji.2020.1345a
Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it’s better to have ability: Evaluative
responses to perceptions of effort and ability. Journal of Research in
Personality, 10(3), 306-315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0092-6566(76)90020-9
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The Development of the concepts of effort and ability,
Perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult
tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49(3), 800. https://doi.
org/10.2307/1128250
Owen, S. V., & Froman, R. D. (1988). Development of a college academic self-
efficacy scale. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the National
Council on Measurement in Education. New Orleans, LA.
Pooresmaeili, A., Wannig, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2015). Receipt of reward leads
to altered estimation of effort. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, 112(43), 13407-13410. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1507527112
Rollwage, M., Pannach, F., Stinson, C., Toelch, U., Kagan, I., & Pooresmaeili, A.
(2020). Judgments of effort exerted by others are influenced by received
rewards. Scientific Reports, 10(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-
58686-0
Schunk, D. H. (1982). Effects of effort attributional feedback on children’s
perceived self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology,
74, 548-556. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.4.548
Schunk, D. H. (1983a). Ability versus effort attributional feedback: Differential
effects on self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology,
75(6), 848-856. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.6.848
Schunk, D. H. (1983b). Reward contingencies and the development of children’s
skills and self-efficacy. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75(4), 511.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.75.4.511
Schunk, D. H. (1984). Enhancing self-efficacy and achievement through
rewards and goals: Motivational and informational effects. The Journal of
Educational Research, 78(1), 29-34. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.198
4.10885568
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational
psychologist, 26(3-4), 207-231. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1991.96
53133
593
Schwarzer, R., & Jerusalem, M. (1995). Generalized self-efficacy scale. In J.
Weinman, S. Wright, & M. Johnston (Eds.), Measures in health psychology:
A user’s portfolio. Causal and control beliefs, 35-37. Windsor, UK: NFER-
NELSON.
Seyed, S., Salmani, M., Motahari Nezhad, F., & Noruzi, R. (2017). Self-efficacy,
achievement motivation, and academic progress of students with learning
disabilities: A comparison with typical students. Middle East Journal of
Rehabilitation and Health, 4(2). https://doi.org/10.5812/mejrh.44558
The jamovi project (2021). jamovi. (Version 1.8) [Computer Software]. Retrieved
from https://www.jamovi.org.
Tzur, S. K., Ganzach, Y., & Pazy, A. (2016). On the positive and negative effects of
self-efficacy on performance: Reward as a moderator. Human Performance,
29(5), 362-377. https://doi.org/10.1080/08959285.2016.1192631
Truong, T. N. N., & Wang, C. (2019). Understanding Vietnamese college students’
self-efficacy beliefs in learning English as a foreign language. System, 84,
123-132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2019.06.007
Van Dinther, M., Dochy, F., & Segers, M. (2011). Factors affecting students’ self-
efficacy in higher education. Educational Research Review, 6(2), 95-108.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2010.10.003
Woulfe, J. (2008). Self-efficacy and culture: A comparison of Denmark and the
United States. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 17-29.
Xing, S., Gao, X., Jiang, Y., Archer, M., & Liu, X. (2018b). Effects of ability
and effort praise on children’s failure attribution, self-handicapping,
and performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 9. https://doi.org/10.3389/
fpsyg.2018.01883
Zuffianò, A., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., Luengo Kanacri, B. P., di Giunta, L.,
Milioni, M., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). Academic achievement: The unique
contribution of self-efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning beyond
intelligence, personality traits, and self-esteem. Learning and Individual
Differences, 23, 158-162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.010
Các file đính kèm theo tài liệu này:
- loi_khen_nang_luc_va_loi_khen_no_luc_doi_voi_su_tu_tin_vao_n.pdf